sieg-mu08

download sieg-mu08

of 5

Transcript of sieg-mu08

  • 7/29/2019 sieg-mu08

    1/5

    Research Report

    Chinese Children Excel on Novel

    Mathematics Problems EvenBefore Elementary SchoolRobert S. Siegler and Yan Mu

    Carnegie Mellon University

    ABSTRACTKindergartners in China showed greater nu-

    merical knowledge than their age peers in the United

    States, not only when tested with arithmetic problems,which Chinese parents present to their children more often

    than U.S. parents do, but also when tested with number-

    line estimation problems, which were novel to the children

    in both countries. The Chinese kindergartners number-

    line estimates were comparable to those of U.S. children 1

    to 2 years more advanced in school. Individual differences

    in arithmetic and number-line-estimation performance

    were positively correlated within each country. These re-

    sults indicate that performance differences between Chi-

    nese and U.S. children on both practiced and unpracticed

    mathematical tasks are substantial even before the chil-

    dren begin elementary school.

    Children in China, Japan, and other East Asian countries out-

    perform their American age peers on numerous mathematical

    tasks, including those involving counting, arithmetic, algebra,

    and geometry (Ginsburg Choi, Lopez, Netley, & Chi, 1997;

    Stevenson, Chen, & Lee, 1993). This learning gap, evident as

    early as kindergarten, persists throughout elementary and high

    school, and probably beyond (Stevenson & Stigler, 1992).

    Most explanations of the learning gap have focused on

    schooling. Children in East Asia spend more time on math in

    classrooms, devote more time to doing math homework after

    school, and encounter more challenging math problems in each

    grade than do their American peers (Chen & Stevenson, 1989;

    Stigler & Hiebert, 1999). Compared with American teachers,

    East Asian teachers more deeply understand fundamental math-

    ematics concepts and use more focused pedagogical practices

    (Ma, 1999), provide more substantive explanations of procedures

    (Perry, 2000), and more often promote multiple solutions to a

    given problem (Geary, 1994).Differing cultural emphases also seem to contribute to the

    learning gap. Parents in China place greater emphasis on the

    importance of mathematics and are more involved in their

    childrens math learning, compared with parents in the United

    States (Huntsinger, Jose, Liaw, & Ching, 1997; Zhou et al.,

    2006). Even before Chinese children enter school, their skill in

    counting and in adding numbers with sums of 10 or less is su-

    perior to that of their American peers (Geary, Bow-Thomas, Fan,

    & Siegler, 1993; Geary, Bow-Thomas, Liu, & Siegler, 1996).

    The basic question that motivated the present study was

    whether the superior mathematical knowledge of East Asian

    preschoolers is limited to skills that are taught directly byparents or is more general. The skills on which cross-national

    differences among preschoolers have been documented

    counting and addingare ones for which Chinese parents

    provide substantial practice before their children begin ele-

    mentary school (Zhou et al., 2006). However, Chinese parents do

    not provide instruction on other, less routine mathematical

    tasks, such as numerical estimation (Zhou et al., 2006). This

    raises the issue of whether the learning gap among young chil-

    dren is merely a superficial result of rote learning, or whether the

    differences in mathematical knowledge between Chinese and

    U.S. children before they begin elementary school extend to the

    ability to solve novel problems.In the present study, we examined Chinese and American

    kindergartners knowledge of a practiced and an unpracticed

    task. The practiced task was arithmetic (addition of single-digit

    numbers). The unpracticed task was number-line estimation

    (Siegler & Opfer, 2003). For each problem on this task, children

    were given a fresh number line with 0 at one end, 100 at the

    other, and nothing in between, and the experimenter asked them

    to locate the position of a number on the number line. The

    logarithmic and linear functions that best fit each childs esti-

    Address correspondence to Robert S. Siegler, Department of Psy-

    chology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213, e-mail:

    [email protected].

    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

    Volume 19Number 8 759Copyrightr

    2008 Association for Psychological Science

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/29/2019 sieg-mu08

    2/5

    mates were determined, and the success of these functions in

    accounting for the estimates of all presented numbers was

    compared.

    For performance on the number-line task to be optimal, the

    best-fitting linear function must account for 100% of variance in

    the estimates and have a slope of 1.00. American adults per-

    formance closely approximates this ideal (Siegler & Opfer,

    2003). In contrast, American children exhibit a consistent age-

    related progression from logarithmic estimation patterns to

    linear ones. On number lines from 0 to 100, the large majority of

    kindergartners generate estimates best fit by a logarithmic

    function, the large majority of second graders generate estimates

    best fit by a linear function, and first graders are about as likely

    to generate one pattern as to generate the other (Geary, Hoard,

    Byrd-Craven, Nugent, & Numtee, 2007; Siegler & Booth, 2004).

    This means that kindergartners and many first graders esti-

    mates of numerical magnitude rise too quickly at the low end of

    the scale and are too compressed at the high end, unlike second

    graders estimates (see Fig. 1). The developmental pattern re-

    peats itself between second and fourth grade in the range from 0to 1,000 (Booth & Siegler, 2006).

    Linear representations of numerical magnitudes appear to be

    crucial to childrens mathematics performance and learning.

    The linearity of individual childrens number-line estimates

    correlates strongly with their performance on numerous other

    mathematical tasks (Booth & Siegler, 2006; Laski & Siegler,

    2007). Linearity of estimates also correlates substantially with

    overall scores on math achievement tests from kindergarten

    through fourth grade (Booth & Siegler, 2006; Siegler & Booth,

    2004). Perhaps most striking is the finding that providing

    randomly selected children with visual representations of the

    magnitudes of addends and sums along a number line improves

    their learning of answers to arithmetic problems (Booth &

    Siegler, 2008).

    The current study is the first to examine Chinese kinder-

    gartners proficiency on the number-line-estimation task. We

    hypothesized that Chinese preschoolers practice at arithmetic

    and counting would improve their understanding of numerical

    magnitudes, even on unpracticed tasks such as number-line

    estimation. Consider how the most common early addition

    strategy, counting fingers, could contribute to knowledge of

    numerical magnitudes. With this strategy, thelarger the sum, the

    more fingers children put up, the more fingers they see, the more

    numbers they count, and the more time they take to reach the

    sum. For example, solving 4 1 4 by counting fingers requires

    putting up and seeing twice as many fingers, saying twice as

    many numbers, and taking roughly twice as much time as

    counting fingers to solve 2 1 2. These kinesthetic, visual, ver-

    bal, and temporal cues provide broad-based support for a sense

    of numerical magnitudes. Thus, the greater arithmetic and

    counting experience of Chinese children, relative to U.S. chil-

    dren, was expected to produce superior number-line estimation,despite the fact that neither Chinese nor U.S. children have

    experience with this task. The same logic implies that within

    each society, individual differences in arithmetic skill and

    number-line estimation should correlate positively: Children

    who are more experienced and skilled at arithmetic should be

    better at the unpracticed number-line task as well.

    Thus, this experiment tested three main predictions: (a) that

    Chinese kindergartners number-line estimates would be more

    accurate, be more linear, and have slopes closer to 1.00 than

    those of American peers; (b) that Chinese kindergartners

    arithmetic skill would exceed that of their American peers; and

    (c)that in both the Chinese andthe American samples,individual

    0

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    0

    Actual Magnitude

    Kindergarten

    0

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    Actual Magnitude

    Second Grade

    Estim

    atedMagnitude

    20 40 60 80 100 0 20 40 60 80 100

    Fig. 1. Median estimates of U.S. kindergartners and second graders as a function of actual magnitude (data from Siegler &

    Booth, 2004, Experiment 1). As the equations show, kindergartners estimates increase logarithmically with numerical

    magnitude, whereas second graders estimates increase linearly.

    760 Volume 19Number 8

    Chinese Children Excel at Math Before Elementary School

  • 7/29/2019 sieg-mu08

    3/5

    childrens number-line proficiency would correlate positively

    with their arithmetic skill.

    METHOD

    Participants

    The 29 Chinese children (mean age5 68 months, range5 63

    73 months; 11 girls, 18 boys) were recruited from a preschoolaffiliated with Tianjin Normal University. The 24 American

    children (mean age 5 67 months, range 5 6375 months; 12

    girls, 12 boys; 88% Caucasian, 12% Asian) were recruited from

    a preschool affiliated with Carnegie Mellon University. Con-

    versations with principals and teachers at the two sites indicated

    that the socioeconomic status of the Chinese children was no

    higher, and was perhaps lower, than that of the American chil-

    dren, and that the prestige of the preschool in China, relative to

    other Chinese preschools, was lower than the prestige of the

    preschool in America, relative to other American preschools.

    The principals and teachers descriptions were consistent with

    the knowledge of the second author, who had lived in both citiesand was familiar with both preschools.

    Procedure

    The problems and instructions on the number-line task were like

    those in previous studies (e.g., Siegler & Booth, 2004). Among

    the 26 numbers whose positions children estimated, 4 were from

    each of the first three decades, and 2 were from each subsequent

    decade. The reason for oversampling the first three decades was

    to better discriminate between linear and logarithmic estimation

    patterns. The numbers3, 4, 6, 8, 12, 14, 17, 18, 21, 24, 25, 29,

    33, 39, 42, 48, 52, 57, 61, 64, 72, 79, 81, 84, 90, and 96were

    ordered randomly.

    The addition task was one used previously by Geary et al.

    (1996) to compare the arithmetic knowledge of Chineseand U.S.

    kindergartners. It consisted of 70 addition problems with sums

    between 2 and 10. Children were given 1 min to answer as many

    items as possible. Thetwo tasks were presented in random order.

    RESULTS

    Number-Line Task

    To examine the accuracy of the childrens estimates on the

    number-line task, we computed their percentage absolute error

    (PAE) using the following formula: PAE 5 jestimate esti-

    mated quantityj/scale of estimates. Thus, if a child was pre-sented the number 85 and estimated its location as being at the

    point that corresponded to 75, the childs PAE on that trial would

    be 10% (j75 85j/100).The American childrens mean PAE was 22% (SD5 8.3). The

    Chinese children were considerably more accurate, with a mean

    PAE of 15% (SD5 7.7), t(51)5 3.05, p< .01, d5 0.82, prep5

    .977. The Chinese kindergartners mean PAE was between thePAEs of American first graders and second graders in previous

    studies (e.g., Siegler & Booth, 2004).

    We next examined the linearity of the childrens estimates. As

    in previous studies, American kindergartners median number-

    line estimates of the 26 numbers magnitudes were better fit by a

    logarithmic function (R25 .90) than by a linear function (R25

    .72; see Fig. 2). Unlike the estimates of any previous American

    kindergarten sample, the Chinese kindergartners median esti-

    mates were better fit by a linear function (R2 5 .95) than by a

    logarithmic function (R25 .86; see Fig. 2).

    Analyses of individual childrens estimates told a similar

    story. For the Chinese sample, the best-fitting linear function

    0

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    00

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    0

    Actual Magnitude

    ChinaU.S.

    Estim

    atedMagnitude

    20 40 60 80 100 20 40 60 80 100

    Actual Magnitude

    Fig. 2. Results from the present study: median estimates of U.S. and Chinese kindergartners as a function of actual

    magnitude. Also shown are thefunctions with thebest fit to thedata (logarithmic forU.S. kindergartners, linearfor Chinese

    kindergartners).

    Volume 19Number 8 761

    Robert S. Siegler and Yan Mu

  • 7/29/2019 sieg-mu08

    4/5

    accounted for greater variance in individualchildrens estimates

    than the best-fitting logarithmic function did (means of 63% vs.

    58%), t(28)5 2.83, p< .01, d5 0.54, prep5 .959. In contrast,

    in the American sample, the best-fitting logarithmic function

    accounted for greater variance in individualchildrens estimates

    than the best-fitting linear function did (means of 53% vs. 43%),

    t(23)5 3.89, p< .01, d5 0.65, prep5 .992. The mean variance

    accounted for by the linear function was greater in the Chinese

    sample than in the American sample (63% vs. 43%), t(51) 5

    2.22, p < .05, d 5 0.65, prep 5 .908. More Chinese than

    American students estimation patterns were better fit by a lin-

    ear function than by a logarithmic one (62% vs. 17%), w2(1,N5

    53)5 11.15, p < .01.

    The slopes of the best-fitting linear functions also tended to be

    closer to 1.00 for the Chinese than for the American children

    (0.53 vs. 0.39), t(51)5 1.85,p< .10, d5 0.50,prep5 .85. Thus,

    analyses of individual childrens accuracy, of the linearity of

    their estimates, and of the slopes of their best-fitting functions

    converged on the conclusion that the Chinese kindergartners

    performance on the novel number-line task was considerablymore advanced than that of the American kindergartners.

    Arithmetic Performance and Its Relation to Number-Line

    Estimation

    The Chinese kindergartners also answered more addition

    problems correctly than did their American peers (M5 8.4,

    SD 5 3.8, vs. M5 5.3, SD 5 4.2), t(51) 5 2.91, p < .01,

    d 5 0.88, prep5 .97.

    Within both the U.S. and the Chinese samples, individual

    differences in the number of correct addition answers were re-

    lated to individual differences in the quality of number-line

    estimates, both when estimation quality was indexed by PAE,rU.S.(22)5 .64, p < .01, and rChina(27)5 .38, p < .05, and

    when estimation quality was indexed by linearity, rU.S.(22) 5

    .60, p < .01, and rChina(27)5 .40, p < .05.

    DISCUSSION

    Results of this study were consistent with our three main hy-

    potheses. First, Chinese kindergartners number-line estimates

    were more advanced than those of their American peers. Second,

    Chinese kindergartners arithmetic performance was also more

    advanced. Third, individual differences in number-line esti-

    mation and arithmetic proficiency were positively correlatedwithin each country. Thus, even before Chinese children enter

    elementary school, their mathematical knowledge is superior on

    tasks that parents do not present, as well as on tasks that parents

    do present, and individual differences in their performance on a

    familiar numerical task are related to individual differences in

    their performance on an unfamiliar numerical task.

    The literature on intersensory redundancy and learning pro-

    vides a useful context for thinking about the present findings.

    Providing multiple, synchronous, redundant cues promotes

    many types of learning, including numerical learning (Jordan,

    Suanda, & Brannon, 2008). For example, Jordan et al. found that

    providing redundant visual and auditory cues to numberallowed

    6-month-olds to make finer discriminations among sets with

    varying ratios of dots than they did when visual or auditory cues

    were presented alone. Our predictions that Chinese childrens

    numerical estimation would be more advanced than American

    childrens and that individual differences in arithmetic profi-

    ciency and number-line estimation would be correlated were

    based on a similar logic. The well-practiced activities of

    counting fingers and other objects to solve arithmetic problems

    and determine set sizes convey redundant kinesthetic, visual,

    auditory, and temporal information about numerical magni-

    tudes. Practice in adding and counting is surely not the only

    source of differences between Chinese and U.S. childrens nu-

    merical knowledge, but it appears to be one source.

    A similar analysis gave rise to a previous prediction that

    playing a numerical board game akin to Chutes and Ladders

    would improve preschoolers knowledge of numerical magni-

    tudes. Such games provide the same type of redundant cues tonumerical magnitude as does counting fingers to solve arith-

    metic problems. The greaterthe number in a square of the board

    game, the greater the number of movements to reach it, the

    greater the number of words the child has said and heard by

    that time, the further the distance of the square from the

    origin, and the more time it takes the token to reach the square.

    In studies consistent with this analysis, preschoolers from low-

    income backgrounds who were randomly assigned to play a

    linear number board game improved their number-line estima-

    tion and magnitude comparison more than did children who

    played a similar game on a board with different colors rather

    than different numbers in the squares (Ramani & Siegler,2008; Siegler & Ramani, in press). As these studies and

    the present study illustrate, analyzing everyday activities in

    terms of the cues they provide for inducing desired concepts

    may advance understanding of cross-cultural, individual,

    developmental, and social-class differences in knowledge and

    learning.

    AcknowledgmentsThis research was supported by Depart-

    ment of Education Grants R305H020060 and R305H050035.

    We thank Yajing Zhang, principal of the preschool affiliated with

    Tianjin Normal University, and Sharon Carver, principal of the

    Carnegie Mellon Childrens School, for their assistance.

    REFERENCES

    Booth, J.L., & Siegler, R.S. (2006). Developmental and individual

    differences in pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psy-

    chology, 41, 189201.

    Booth, J.L., & Siegler, R.S. (2008). Numerical magnitude represen-

    tations influence arithmetic learning. Child Development, 79,

    10161031.

    762 Volume 19Number 8

    Chinese Children Excel at Math Before Elementary School

  • 7/29/2019 sieg-mu08

    5/5

    Chen, C., & Stevenson, H.W. (1989). Homework: A cross-cultural

    examination. Child Development, 60, 551561.

    Geary, D.C. (1994). Childrens mathematical development: Research

    and practical applications. Washington, DC: American Psycho-

    logical Association.

    Geary, D.C., Bow-Thomas, C.C., Fan, L., & Siegler, R.S. (1993). Even

    before formal instruction, Chinese children outperform American

    children in mental addition. Cognitive Development, 8, 517529.

    Geary, D.C., Bow-Thomas, C.C., Liu, F., & Siegler, R.S. (1996). De-velopment of arithmetical competencies in Chinese and Ameri-

    can children: Influence of age, language, and schooling. Child

    Development, 67, 20222044.

    Geary, D.C., Hoard, M.K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, C.

    (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement deficits in

    children with mathematical learning disability. Child Develop-

    ment, 78, 13431359.

    Ginsburg, H.C., Choi, Y.E., Lopez, L.S., Netley, R., & Chi, C.-Y.

    (1997). Happy birthday to you: Early mathematical thinking of

    Asian, South American and U.S. children. In T. Nunes &

    P. Bryant (Ed.), Learning and teaching mathematics: An inter-

    national perspective (pp. 163207). Hove, England: Psychology

    Press.

    Huntsinger, C.S., Jose, P.E., Liaw, F.R., & Ching, W. (1997). Culturaldifferences in early mathematics learning: A comparison of Euro-

    American, Chinese-American, and Taiwan-Chinese families.

    International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 371388.

    Jordan, K.E., Suanda, S.H., & Brannon, E.M. (2008). Intersensory

    redundancy accelerates preverbal numerical competence. Cog-

    nition, 108, 210221.

    Laski, E.V., & Siegler, R.S. (2007). Is 27 a big number? Correlational

    and causal connections among numerical categorization, number

    line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child

    Development, 76, 17231743.

    Ma, L. (1999). Knowing and teaching elementary mathematics:

    Teachers understanding of fundamental mathematics in China

    and the United States. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Perry, M. (2000). Explanations of mathematical concepts in Japanese,

    Chinese and U.S. first- and fifth-grade classrooms. Cognition and

    Instruction, 18, 181207.

    Ramani, G.B., & Siegler, R.S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable

    improvements in low-income childrens numerical knowledge

    through playing number board games. Child Development, 79,375394.

    Siegler, R.S., & Booth, J.L. (2004). Development of numerical esti-

    mation in young children. Child Development, 75, 428444.

    Siegler, R.S., & Opfer, J.E. (2003). The development of numerical

    estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical

    quantity. Psychological Science, 14, 237243.

    Siegler, R.S., & Ramani, G.B. (in press). Playing board games pro-

    motes low-income childrens numerical development. Develop-

    mental Science.

    Stevenson, H.W., Chen, C., & Lee, S.Y. (1993). Mathematics

    achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children: Ten

    years later. Science, 259, 5358.

    Stevenson, H.W., & Stigler, J.W. (1992). The learning gap. New York:

    Free Press.Stigler, J.W., & Hiebert, J. (1999). The teaching gap: Best ideas from

    the worlds teachers for improving education in the classroom . New

    York: Free Press.

    Zhou, X., Huang, J., Wang, Z., Wang, B., Zhao, Z., Yang, L., & Zheng-

    zheng, Y. (2006). Parent-child interaction and childrens number

    learning. Early Child Development and Care, 176, 763775.

    (RECEIVED 1/11/08; REVISION ACCEPTED 3/27/08)

    Volume 19Number 8 763

    Robert S. Siegler and Yan Mu